1. Home
  2. Farm Animals
  3. Cattle
  4. Beef Cattle
  5. What is virtual fencing (and virtual herding) and does it impact animal welfare?
  1. Home
  2. Farm Animals
  3. General Farming Practices
  4. Management
  5. What is virtual fencing (and virtual herding) and does it impact animal welfare?
  1. Home
  2. Farm Animals
  3. Cattle
  4. Dairy Cattle
  5. What is virtual fencing (and virtual herding) and does it impact animal welfare?
  1. Home
  2. Farm Animals
  3. Sheep
  4. What is virtual fencing (and virtual herding) and does it impact animal welfare?
Print

What is virtual fencing (and virtual herding) and does it impact animal welfare?

What is virtual fencing?

Virtual fencing aims to remotely map and control livestock grazing behaviour without the use of fixed fences [1]. This technology, which was first developed in 2005, uses Global Positioning System (GPS) sensors and wireless technologies to control the movement and location of animals within an area [2].

How does virtual fencing work?

For virtual fencing to work, animals need to be fitted with a GPS collar. The collar emits a tone (audio cue) and an electric shock. As the animal approaches a pre-determined virtual barrier, the GPS collar emits an audio cue. If the animal continues towards the virtual barrier, the animal receives an aversive electric shock [3, 4]. The virtual fencing system requires that animals respond appropriately to the audio cue (i.e. move in a different direction) to avoid receiving an electric shock. Although the method of delivery of the electric shock is not directly comparable to that of an electric fence, the intensity of the shock delivered by the collar worn by cattle is less than that of an electric fence [5]. For sheep, some studies have used a modified and manually operated shock collar designed for dogs [6].

Virtual fencing has the potential to improve livestock, pasture and environmental management and reduce labour and costs associated with fixed fences. However, the use of electric shocks to train an animal raises significant ethical and animal welfare concerns. For example, what are the potential welfare implications to animals who don’t understand and are unable to learn quickly? Or for animals who never learn? Are there implications on animals’ mental state?

What is virtual herding?

Virtual herding is the use of a virtual fencing system to move animals from one location to another, rather than simply containing them to one area. This can be done using another cue such as vibration, which is a signal for the animal to move forward. If the animal does not respond in the desired way to the vibration cues, an electric shock is administered to the animal. Virtual herding adds complexity to what animals need to learn in order to avoid shocks, compared with static virtual fencing. There is very little research published on the welfare impacts of virtual herding. One study found that it took dairy cows longer to learn how to avoid the electric shock during virtual herding compared with virtual fencing [7]. A recent independent literature review commissioned by the Australian Government recommended that, in order to minimise negative welfare impacts, virtual fencing technology should not be deployed with rapidly moving boundaries [8].

What are the animal welfare implications of virtual fencing?

Although there are claims that virtual fencing is an animal-friendly technology [2], there is a lack of research regarding the long-term impacts of this technology on animal welfare.​ ​

One of the main welfare concerns of virtual fencing is the use of an electric shock, which, in order for the technology to be effective, must be sufficiently aversive that animals actively try to avoid it. One study found that sheep showed more avoidance behaviours towards an electrical stimulus than they did to a barking dog, and that behaviours in response to the electrical stimulus were more extreme, such as rearing, jumping or falling [9]. Farm animals can find restraint psychologically stressful, and cattle have shown a similar physiological and behavioural response to an electric shock compared with being restrained in a crush [10]. One experiment found that cattle confined behind a virtual fence spent less time lying down than those confined with electric tape [5]. Although the difference in lying time was small, this could indicate that virtually fenced cows are less relaxed or comfortable compared with those contained with electric tape.

Another welfare concern of this technology is that it initially exposes animals to an environment of low predictability and controllability, which can lead to anxiety and psychological stress during the learning period [4]. During the initial phase of training, animals do not know what the audio cue means, and therefore cannot avoid receiving the electric shock(s) [4]. During the learning and adaptation phase, animals may be in an aroused state and experience psychological stress for several hours to several days until they have learnt to link the audio cue with the electric shock and understand how to respond, i.e. move away, in order not to receive a shock [11]. Learning is successful when an animal is able to predict and control how they interact with the virtual fence.

As the virtual fence is not visible, it may be more difficult for some animals to learn compared with an electric fence, as there are no visual cues to guide them. Studies have reported that cattle learn to respond to the audio cue after 1- 6 electric shocks, but high individual variation has been observed in animals’ rates of learning [1, 3, 5]. A virtual herding project’s final report mentions that across many of the trials, once animals were trained, nearly 90% responded to the audio cue and avoided receiving an electric shock. This suggests that some animals may learn much slower or may not learn at all, putting them at a higher risk of compromised welfare. Animals with prior exposure to an electric fence appear to learn to respond faster to the audio cue [13]. Social learning also appears to play a role, with both cattle and sheep in small experimental groups following a herd mate who they’d seen move away from the virtual fence [14, 15]. Further studies are needed to investigate the long-term effects of virtual fencing on the physiological and behavioural response of animals.

Other welfare concerns of virtual fencing relate to potential irritation and/or ulceration of the skin due to the use of collars, choking on collars, equipment malfunction [1], and deliberate or accidental misuse.

What laws control the use of virtual fencing in Australia?

State and territory animal welfare legislation determines where and what type of electronic devices can or cannot be used to contain livestock. Electronic collars have been trialled on farms in Queensland, New South Wales and Tasmania as part of the Department of Agriculture and Water Resources Rural Research and Development for Profit Program (for more information see the final report). Currently, the collars cannot be used commercially in Victoria, South Australia, New South Wales or the Australian Capital Territory. However, exemption permits can be obtained for research purposes. In Victoria for example, electronic collars can be used on cattle, sheep, goats, pigs, camels, alpacas or llamas as part of a scientific procedure, or a program of scientific procedures, approved under a licence granted under Part 3 of the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals Act 1986. In Western Australia, the Animal Welfare (General) Regulations 2003 were changed in June 2022 and now permit the use of one brand of virtual fencing technology in cattle, as long as the device is used in accordance with the manufacturer’s instructions. Virtual fencing of livestock is permitted in the Northern Territory, Tasmania and Queensland.

What is the RSPCA’s view?

The RSPCA has concerns about the use of electronically activated devices that deliver an electric shock to animals, as these are aversive and use punishment-based training principles. RSPCA supports the use of humane husbandry, management and training practices that do not cause pain, injury, suffering or distress. Where virtual fencing technology is legally permitted, the use of collars should be subject to regulation that ensures safeguards are in place to protect the welfare of animals. This includes adherence to standards that ensure appropriate use and avoid animal harm, as well as third-party monitoring/auditing to verify compliance.

References

[1] Umstatter C (2011) The evolution of virtual fences: A review. Comput Electron Agric75(1):10–22.

[2] CSIRO (2024) Virtual fencing. https://www.csiro.au/en/research/technology-space/it/Virtual-fencing. Accessed 29 May 2024.

[3] Campbell DLM et al (2018) Virtual fencing of cattle using an automated collar in a feed attractant trial. Appl Anim Behav Sci 200:71–77.

[4] Lee C, Colditz IG, Campbell DL (2018) A framework to assess the impact of new animal management technologies on welfare: A case study of virtual fencing. Front Vet Sci 5:187.

[5] Campbell DLM, Lea JM, Keshavarzi H et al (2019) Virtual fencing is comparable to electric tape fencing for cattle behavior and welfare. Front Vet Sci 6, 445.

[6] Marini D, Cowley F, Belson S et al (2019) The importance of an audio cue warning in training sheep to a virtual fence and differences in learning when tested individually or in small groups. Appl Anim Behav Sci 104862

[7] Verdon M, Hunt I, Rawnsley R (in press) The effectiveness of a virtual fencing technology to allocate pasture and herd cows to the milking shed. J Dairy Sci. https://doi.org/10.3168/jds.2023-24537

[8] Fisher A, Cornish A (2022) Independent scientific literature review on animal welfare considerations for virtual fencing. Report for Department of Agriculture, Fisheries and Forestry. https://www.agriculture.gov.au/agriculture-land/animal/welfare/awtg. Accessed 29 May 2024.

[9] Kearton T, Marini D, Cowley F et al (2019) The effect of virtual fencing stimuli on stress responses and behavior in sheep. Animals 9, 30.

[10] Lee C, Fisher AD, Reed MT et al (2008) The effect of low energy electric shock on cortisol, beta-endorphin, heart rate and behaviour of cattle. Appl Anim Behav Sci 113:32–42.

[11] Lee C, Campbell DLM (2021) A multi-disciplinary approach to assess the welfare impacts of a new virtual fencing technology. Front Vet Sci 8, 637709.

[12] Lee C et al (2009) Associative learning by cattle to enable effective and ethical virtual fences. Appl Anim Behav Sci119(1–2):15–22.

[13] Verdon M, Lee C, Marini D et al (2020) Pre-exposure to an electrical stimulus primes associative pairing of audio and electrical stimuli for dairy heifers in a virtual fencing feed attractant trial. Animals 10(2), 217.

[14] Keshavarzi H, Lee C, Lea JM et al (2020) Virtual fence responses are socially facilitated in beef cattle. Front Vet Sci 7, 543158.

[15] Marini D, Kearton T, Ouzman J et al (2020) Social influence on the effectiveness of virtual fencing in sheep. PeerJ 8, e10066.

Also Read

Updated on July 2, 2024
  • Home
  • Farm Animals
  • Cattle
  • Beef Cattle
  • Home
  • Farm Animals
  • General Farming Practices
  • Management
  • Home
  • Farm Animals
  • Cattle
  • Dairy Cattle
  • Home
  • Farm Animals
  • Sheep
https://kb.rspca.org.au/knowledge-base/what-is-virtual-fencing-and-virtual-herding-and-does-it-impact-animal-welfare/

Was this article helpful?